Utility and validity
A frequent concern
is, if psychiatric disorders don’t actually describe “natural kinds” i.e.
clearly separate illnesses such as differentiating pneumonia from a heart
attack then this lack of validity means they have no usefulness in treatment or
prognosis (utility).
Think of colours
Western and Japanese
culture recognises 7 main colours (excluding black and white) other cultures
recognise smaller number e.g. some in
Africa recognise 2 main colours. When tested all people from all cultures can
recognise differences in tone/ colour when tested. (So clearly separate
different colours can be reliably recognised).
Differences between
colours are not absolute e.g. orange eventually becomes yellow but the borderline
is indistinct. Differences between neighbour colours is more difficult e.g.
orange/yellow or indigo/ violet than differences between clearly separate
colours e.g. orange/violet.
So this is a spectrum
phenomenon (but with an external validated criterion of light frequency/wavelength)
that we categorise into separate colours, not by the external criterion but how
it appears to us (so the wavelength is assigned to a subjectively decided
colour). The number of categories is culturally determined and we can reliably
differentiate between colours/ tones if on clearly different points of spectrum
but not if close together on spectrum.
We categorise colours
despite it actually being a spectrum because we find it useful for purposes of
description. We categorise based on archetypes e.g. of orange, of yellow, not
clearly separated. The archetype is of the greenest green or the bluest blue
etc Note there is no assumption of “clustering” i.e. that in nature, light
frequencies/wavelengths encountered will be more common either each named
archetype/ category or that the prevalence of light frequency/wavelengths will
be commoner in the centre of the archetype/categories light frequency/
wavelength optimum value.
Note Pantone system
of allocating number and name codes to varying shades of colour is a form of
categorisation using archetypes.
Application to mental
disorders
If you assume that
mental activities/ behaviours/ symptoms exist on a spectrum (multi-dimensional
rather than single dimensional) and are not separated dichotomously from each
other or from “normal” range (i.e. are not separate “natural kinds” and not
valid separate categories)
You can still apply
categories to these dimensions in a similar way to colours above.
These categories form
“archetypes” which to be useful need to be
- Reliably distinguished (neighbour
categories harder to distinguish than distant categories)
- Provide useful information in terms of
other associated features, prognostic frequencies (not absolute but
increased and decreased rates), guide to treatment based on research, use
for stats etc
- We can expect that differences between
“neighbouring” archetypes may be less than those with “distant” archetypes
- That marginal cases may exist that
don’t fit closely into these archetypes (c.f. index levels of caseness in
S.C.A.N. interview schedule) but that the optimum number of categories is
not necessarily fixed but depends on other factors (c.f. different no. of
colours in different cultures) see debates between lumpers and splitters
Now how might this
work in practise?
Looking at 2
dimensions: psychotic symptoms and presence/absence of mania. Now some cases of
psychosis may have mild manic type symptoms intermittently but overall are
closer to the psychosis, no mania archetype. Let’s place them at different part
of the spectrum (equivalent to red and violet in he rainbow). Let’s look at one
aspect: the effectiveness of Lithium and antipsychotics in treating the
presenting picture. We can get the following picture (derived from Cochrane
Reviews and clinical practise).
Category
|
Antipsychotic effective?
|
Lithium Effective?
|
Mania, no psychosis
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Mania with psychosis
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Psychosis, no/little mania
|
Yes
|
No
|
Normal
|
No
|
No
|
I’ve not described
some of the other differences e.g. tendency to different ages of onset,
different social class prevalence, different levels of and type of cognitive
impairment etc.
We can see from the
point of view of a prescriber it is important to distinguish presence of mania
and psychosis and to allocate to appropriate category (one can argue the
important question is manic yes/no? if no, psychotic yes/ no, give appropriate
treatment. Note for a prescriber the individual symptom in this case is
unimportant, e.g. auditory hallucination, persecutory delusion, insomnia, what
is more important is to which class the symptom in context with other symptoms
places the patient into.
Another important
distinguishing feature could be outcome/prognosis. Very few general medical
illnesses give an absolute prognosis e.g. 100% guaranteed to recur in a
specific time frame. They often give a range of probabilities of outcome.
The AESOP study in
the UK looked at people with a First Episode psychosis. They found that people
who presented with Mania had 3-4 times better chance of symptom and functional
recovery at 3 and 10 years than people presenting with Schizophrenia/
Schizoaffective disorder (a mixture of both psychosis and mood disorder). (From
a lecture given at the General Adult Psychiatry Conference in Manchester in
2013).
So can you reliably
identify different points of the spectrum? After all, if people confuse red for
green what’s the use of a colour system (and of course some people with colour
blindness do have difficulty differentiating certain colours but the prevalence
of this problem is low enough for us to continue using colours for many
purposes).
One way of looking at this is what’s called a
kappa concordance figure. In the UK we use ICD10 as our official diagnostic
system. Field trials showed the kappa figures for schizophrenia overall as a
category were very good 0.77-0.84 and for bipolar, manic disorders to be
0.69-0.77. (kappa figures 0.61-0.8 are regarded as substantial agreement
between separate raters). This suggests that different clinicians have a high
chance of reliably agreeing between themselves what is a manic state and a
psychotic state without mania.
So we can see how
even if psychosis is like a multidimensional spectrum disorder and not divided
into clearly separate disorders then it can still be useful to recognise
different points of the spectrum if they are associated with different
treatment responses and outcomes.
In short, utility
without validity.
However this utility
of making the diagnosis depends on how reliably the diagnosis can be made by
different clinicians and whether there is sufficient difference in treatment
and/or prognosis/outcome for it to be worthwhile. This needs to be made for
each diagnosis.
Also, within this
psychosis spectrum many people don’t come close to either archetype (equivalent
to yellow or green) so assumptions of treatment response and prognosis are less
likely to apply to them.
At this point we
start to get “how-cannery”, how can these broadbrush archetypes actually
predict treatment response, outcomes etc? How can people with such varying
content even within the same symptom (the wide variety of different types of
persecutory delusions for example) let
alone multiple types of symptoms have the same range of probabilities of
response to treatment and outcome? Setting aside the fact that the brain
outputs are by far the most complex of any bodily organ outputs (compare the heart which basically pumps
blood), the fact is that research and clinical practice demonstrates that this
is indeed the case. A hypothetical problem is solved by experience in the real
world. Of course in psychiatry and general medicine also, people with the same
diagnosis have different responses to treatment and outcome.
People not using the medical model often have other ways of describing problems that they find more useful to them when helping people with their problems
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: only a member of this blog may post a comment.